FUTURE
SPORT, EXERCISE AND PHYSICAL EDUCATION
PROFESSIONALS' PERCEPTIONS OF THE PHYSICAL
SELF OF OBESE CHILDREN
By Derek
M. Peters, and Ruan
J.A. Jones
Kinesiology:
International Journal of Fundamental
and Applied Kinesiology (2010)
42(1): 36-43. The journal abstract and
full length article can be downloaded
at: Hrcak
Portal of scientific journals of Croatia.
Introduction
The World Health Organization has described
the increasing prevalence of obesity
across all age groups in the world as
a ‘global epidemic’ (WHO,
2006). In the UK alone, there
are estimated to be around 1 million
obese children under the age of 16 with
the prevalence of ‘at least overweight’
in boys and girls aged 2-15 in 2002
reported as 22 and 28%, respectively
(National Office
for Statistics, 2002). The trend
in childhood obesity and the identification
of the potential importance of sport
and physical activity participation
in reducing the problem has stimulated
a range of cooperative multi-agency
initiatives in England. These have involved
sports coaches, exercise specialists
and physical educators as the frontline
physical activity promoters to maximize
sport, exercise and physical activity
participation throughout childhood and
youth (The Audit
Commission, 2006; Department of Health,
2008).
Previous
research has, however, identified anti-fat
bias amongst such practitioners and
indicated that negative implicit and
explicit attitudes and perceptions can
predict prejudiced behaviour towards
the target groups (McConnell
& Leibold, 2001; Rudman & Glick,
2001). Indeed, in the broadest
context, Davison and Birch (2004) conclude
that "discriminatory practices
emanating from fat stereotypes have
broad-reaching negative implications
on the accessibility of health care,
educational opportunities, employment
opportunities, and the social treatment
of overweight individuals."
It is therefore
essential to identify if such negative
perceptions of overweight and obese
children exist in those undergoing training
to become the physical activity professionals
of the future. If such perceptions are
manifest, the findings from this research
would add further support to calls for
the development of awareness raising
interventions in the training of these
populations (Gately,
2007). Anti-fat bias has been
observed in the most knowledgeable obesity
health professionals in the USA, with
stereotypes of 'lazy,' 'stupid,' and
'worthless' associated with obese people
(Schwartz, Chambliss,
Brownell, Blair, & Billington, 2003).
There is some evidence to suggest that
this bias is stronger amongst females,
in younger respondents and those who
had higher body mass index (BMI).
The outcomes
of the study clearly support findings
from previous research which identified
that overweight women perceived that
their health professional did not understand
how difficult it was to be overweight
(Wadden, et al.,
2000). As such, those with a
professional role in supporting patients
and reducing levels of overweight and
obesity were found to possess prejudice
that was explicitly evident to their
patients. It was purported, therefore,
that if patients felt uncomfortable
in such healthcare settings as a result
of practitioner bias, "it would
not be surprising if they avoided care"
and that interventions which enhanced
"appreciation of the experiences
of obese individuals may be useful in
changing attitudes" (Schwartz,
et al., 2003).
A large online
sample of the general population representing
weight category ranges from underweight
to extremely obese has also demonstrated
that whilst all weight status groups
exhibited significant anti-fat bias,
there was an inverse relationship between
weight status of the participant and
the level of observed bias, with weight
bias decreased with increased weight
category (Schwartz,
Vartanian, Nosek, & Brownell, 2006).
Chambliss,
Finley, & Blair (2004) found a strong
anti-fat bias in the 'good' vs 'bad'
and 'motivated' vs 'lazy' implicit stereotype
measures, with having an obese friend,
having a family history of obesity and
lower belief in the level of personal
responsibility for the condition associated
with less anti-fat bias. As such, their
research was congruent with the seemingly
global anti-fat bias, but also acknowledged
that having a direct familial or social
relationship with an obese person, and
holding the belief that the condition
is not purely the individual;s fault
mediated the strength of the anti-fat
bias. This would appear congruent with
the previously cited appreciation of
the experiences of obese individuals
being key to changing anti-fat attitudes
(Schwartz, et
al., 2003) and again, highlights
the potential negative impact that failure
to appreciate obesity issues may have
upon the likelihood of positive interaction
between obese people and the health,
fitness and physical education professionals
of tomorrow.
Anti-fat
attitudes and negative obesity bias
have also been identified specifically
in future physical education, exercise
and fitness professionals through research
involving physical education undergraduates
in New Zealand (O’Brien,
Hunter, & Banks, 2007) and
undergraduate and graduate students
studying exercise sciences in the USA
(Chambliss, et
al., 2004). Research with first
year and third year PE students, and
a control sample of psychology students
from the University of Otago in New
Zealand, demonstrated several significant
differences between the PE and control
sample and between different year groups
of PE students (O’Brien,
et al., 2007).
Whilst all
student groups demonstrated significant
anti-fat bias in three attributes of
the Implicit Association Test in relation
to attitudes of 'good' vs 'bad,' 'motivated'
vs 'lazy,' and 'smart' vs 'fat,' main
effects were found for both the student
group (PE group greater anti-fat bias)
and the year group (year three greater
anti-fat bias), and a significant interaction
effect revealed that the year three
PE sample had significantly greater
anti-fat bias than all other groups
of students.
Whilst the
remainder of the instruments used by
O’Brien et al. (2007) related
to the students’ explicit attitudes
towards measures of physical self-identify
and ideological beliefs rather than
directly to student perceptions of fatness
in children, the authors concluded that
small attitudinal differences between
student groups in the first year of
their course increase greatly over time.
This was suggested as "indicating
the socialization of prejudice"
(O’Brien,
et al., 2007) which would appear
to be heightened in physical education
programmes. Although without the benefit
of data gathered in specific relation
to physical education delivery, as negative
attitudes have previously been identified
as leading to prejudiced behaviours
(McConnell &
Leibold, 2001), O’Brien
et al. (2007) also concluded that the
training of physical educators needs
to include programmes to reduce implicit
and explicit anti-fat prejudice. This
would appear congruent with the contention
of Martinek (1997) who found that lower
expectations of individuals have also
been shown to influence the quality
and quantity of feedback and instruction
in physical activity and sport-related
educational settings.
Surprisingly,
however, only one published study has
examined the perceptions of youth 'obesity'
among practising PE teachers (Greenleaf
& Weiller, 2005). In their
sample of 105 practising PE teachers,
Greenleaf and Weiller (2005) measured
global anti-fat attitudes towards fat
'people' using the Anti-fat
Attitudes Scale (Morrison
& O’Connor, 1999);
performance expectations for 'overweight'
and 'normal weight' youth using selected
strength, flexibility, endurance/fitness,
coordination and sport competence scale
items from Marsh’s
Physical Self-Description Questionnaire;
and overall expectations for 'normal
weight' and 'overweight' youth in relation
to physical skill, reasoning, cooperation
and social interaction.
As such,
though only representing approximately
half of the data collection instruments
used in the research, these aspects
of their investigation related to 'overweight'
rather than obesity issues in youth,
and in relation to global rather than
youth-related anti-fat attitudes. Despite
potential misinterpretation of the focus
of the study upon 'overweight' as opposed
to over 'fatness,' all expectation and
ability scores were significantly worse
for 'overweight' compared with 'normal-weight'
youth.
The items
exhibiting the most equivocal status
between 'overweight' and 'normal-weight'
youth were strength expectation, sport
competence expectation and reasoning
ability, whilst the greatest differentials
were found in the items representing
endurance expectation and physical ability.
It was concluded that holding lower
expectations of, and anticipating decreased
physical ability in, ;over-weight' youth
could be problematic for the provision
of effective physical education programmes.
Thus, research
has shown that current and future health
and PE professionals at the forefront
of obesity prevention and physical activity
promotion show socialized anti-fat bias
that is similar to the population and
control samples. It is also suggested
that the explicit awareness of this
by patients/clients/ students may well
reduce the positive experience and likelihood
of participation in health care, physical
activity promotion and indeed physical
education settings.
Despite this,
no published work has explored future
sport, exercise, and physical education
professionals’ perceptions of
the physical self of the 'obese child'
in comparison with their perception
of the physical self of the 'normal
weight' child. Hence, the present research
is novel in its attempt to quantify
the perceptions of the physical self
of obese children held by future sport,
exercise, and physical education professionals.
This research also provides the first
empirically supported discussion of
the ramifications of the presence of
explicit, anti-fat perceptions potentially
resulting in prejudice, non-inclusive
behaviours, and reduced likelihood of
participation in organized sport, exercise
and physical education opportunities.
The aim of
the research was to investigate the
perceptions held by future sport, exercise
and physical education professionals
of the physical self of 'fat' children
compared with their perceptions of 'normal-weight'
children.
METHODS
Participants
One hundred and sixty-seven students
(mean age 20.9±2.9 yrs; mean
height 1.75±.11 m; mean mass
72.4±12.7 kg; mean BMI 23.7±3.4
kg .m2) who were studying sport, exercise,
and physical education-related courses
in higher education in England completed
a questionnaire consisting of demographic
information and an adapted version of
the Children
and Youth Physical Self Perception Profile
(Whitehead, 1995).
Instruments
The first section of the survey contained
informed consent information including
an explanation of the study, including
the nature of the subsequent questions.
Participation was entirely voluntary
with completed and returned questionnaires
accepted as informed consent to participate.
The second
section of the survey consisted of questions
which gathered demographic data about
the respondent. These included questions
to obtain data for the following variables:
sex, age in years, self-reported height
(m) and weight (kg), course of study
and current year of study (ranging from
first to fourth year). This was followed
by an adapted version of the CY-PSPP.
The CY-PSPP is a 36-item questionnaire
that has been commonly used to measure
the physical self-perceptions of children
and adolescents (Jones,
Polman & Peters, 2009; Whitehead,
1995). The instrument is headed
by the title What am I like?
and requires the participant to determine
which of two opposing statements is
most like them, and then to select one
option to indicate whether the statement
they have selected is really true or
sort of true for them (see Figure 1).
Within the
questionnaire, there are six subscales
that measure self-perceptions of sports
competence (SPORT), physical condition
(CONDITION), body image (BODY), physical
strength (STRENGTH), physical self-worth
(PSW) and global self-esteem (GSE).
The subscale mean was calculated as
the mean of the responses to the six
individual component items that make
up the subscale, with a higher score
representing a higher self-perception.
The adapted CY-PSPP used in the present
study was revised so that each item
had the prefix Compared to normal
weight kids… and had some
kids or other kids
replaced with fat kids in every
item (see Figure 2). This approach permitted
respondents to determine their own perception
of normal-weight kids against which
to compare their perception of fat kids
and as such permitted the comparison
with a self-referenced peer control
group 'norm.' The participants were
required to decide which statement was
true for their perception of fat
kids compared to normal-weight
kids, and then indicate whether
the statement was sort of true or really
true. One example of the adaptation
of an original item for each subscale
follows:
-- |
sport
competence (SPORT) - the original
item
What am I like?..., some kids do
very well at all kinds of sports
OR some kids don’t feel they
are very good when it comes to sports
was adapted to Compared to normal-weight
kids…, fat kids do very well
at all kinds of sports OR fat kids
don’t feel they are very good
when it comes to sports; |
-- |
physical
condition (CONDITION) - the original
item
What am I like?…, some kids
have a lot of stamina for vigorous
physical exercise OR some kids soon
get out of breath and have to slow
down or quit was adapted to Compared
to normal-weight kids…, fat
kids have a lot of stamina for vigorous
physical exercise OR fat kids soon
get out of breath and have to slow
down or quit; |
-- |
body
image (BODY) – the original
item What am I like?...,
some kids are pleased with the appearance
of their bodies OR some kids wish
that their bodies looked in better
shape physically was adapted to
Compared to normal- -weight kids…,
fat kids are pleased with the appearance
of their bodies OR fat kids wish
that their bodies looked in better
shape physically; |
-- |
strength
(STRENGTH) – the original
item
What am I like?..., some kids
think that they are strong, and
have good muscles compared to other
kids their age OR some kids think
that they are weaker, and don’t
have such good muscles as other
kids their age was adapted to Compared
to normal-weight kids…, fat
kids think that they are strong,
and have good muscles compared to
other kids their age OR fat kids
think that they are weaker, and
don’t have such good muscles
as other kids their age; |
-- |
physical
self-worth (PSW) – the original
item
What am I like?..., some kids
have a positive feeling about themselves
physically OR some kids feel somewhat
negative about themselves physically
was adapted to Compared to normal-
weight kids…, fat kids have
a positive feeling about themselves
physically OR fat kids feel somewhat
negative about themselves physically; |
-- |
global
self-esteem (GSE) – the original
item
What am I like?..., some kids
are happy with themselves as a person
OR some kids are often not happy
with themselves was adapted to Compared
to normal-weight kids…, fat
kids are happy with themselves as
a person OR fat kids are often not
happy with themselves. |

|